Skip navigation
  • Home
  • Browse
    • Communities
      & Collections
    • Browse Items by:
    • Publication Date
    • Author
    • Title
    • Subject
    • Department
  • Sign on to:
    • My MacSphere
    • Receive email
      updates
    • Edit Profile


McMaster University Home Page
  1. MacSphere
  2. Open Access Dissertations and Theses Community
  3. Open Access Dissertations and Theses
Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: http://hdl.handle.net/11375/8609
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.advisorJacoby, Larry L.en_US
dc.contributor.authorSte-Marie, Dianeen_US
dc.date.accessioned2014-06-18T16:43:25Z-
dc.date.available2014-06-18T16:43:25Z-
dc.date.created2011-01-06en_US
dc.date.issued1992en_US
dc.identifier.otheropendissertations/3800en_US
dc.identifier.other4817en_US
dc.identifier.other1720539en_US
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/11375/8609-
dc.description.abstract<p>Eight experiments are reported that examined the contrast between spontaneous and directed recognition using the flanker paradigm. The rationale was that spontaneous recognition of a flanking word would be reflected by the influence that word had on recognition of a target word. Spontaneous recognition, as indexed by flanker effects, was found but only under a restricted set of conditions. When attention was divided at test, recognition decisions for target words were faster when the flanker and target word were congruent (old flanker, old target word; new flanker, new target word) rather than incongruent (new flanker, old target word; old flanker, new target word) with regard to the decision they dictated (Exps. 1, 2, 4, 7, 8a, 8b). However, if old targets were easily identified as old, flanker effects did not emerge, even when attention was divided (Exps. 5 and 6). Surprisingly, increasing the number of prior presentations before a word served as a flanker decreased its likelihood to produce effects when old targets were words that had been presented once at study (Exps. 2 and 4). Words that had been presented to be solved as anagrams at study were effective as flankers, despite their change in physical characteristics from study to test (Exp. 7). Further, rather than the form of modality-specific transfer, flankers were more effective if they matched the modality in which old targets had been presented at study (Exps. 8a and 8b). In combination, the results show that it is the relation between the processing history of the target and flanking words, rather than the absolute history of the flanking word, that determines whether flanker effects will be observed. These findings are discussed in terms of the relativity of automaticity (cf., Neumann, 1984).</p>en_US
dc.subjectPsychologyen_US
dc.subjectPsychologyen_US
dc.titleSpontaneous vs directed recognition: The relativity of automaticityen_US
dc.typethesisen_US
dc.contributor.departmentPsychologyen_US
dc.description.degreeDoctor of Philosophy (PhD)en_US
Appears in Collections:Open Access Dissertations and Theses

Files in This Item:
File SizeFormat 
fulltext.pdf
Open Access
1.66 MBAdobe PDFView/Open
Show simple item record Statistics


Items in MacSphere are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.

Sherman Centre for Digital Scholarship     McMaster University Libraries
©2022 McMaster University, 1280 Main Street West, Hamilton, Ontario L8S 4L8 | 905-525-9140 | Contact Us | Terms of Use & Privacy Policy | Feedback

Report Accessibility Issue